Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104081
DOI URL
directSciHub download
Show/hide publication abstract
Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (1999). Beliefs in
conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20, 637–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-
895x.00160.
Abrosoft. (2016). Fantamorph (Version 5.4.7). http://www.fantamorph.com.
Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behavior. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 4, 165–178. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1056.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and
review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 888–918. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888.
Balliet, D., & Van Lange, P. A. (2013). Trust, conflict, and cooperation: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1090–1112. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030939.
Baudson, T. G., & Preckel, F. (2016). Mini-q: Intelligenzscreening in drei Minuten [mini-
q: A three-minute intelligence screening.]. Diagnostica, 62, 182–197. https://doi.org/
10.1026/0012-1924/a000150.
Baumert, A., & Schmitt, M. (2009). Justice-sensitive interpretations of ambiguous
situations. Australian Journal of Psychology, 61, 6–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00049530802607597.
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games
and Economic Behavior, 10, 122–142. https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1995.1027.
Bonnefon, J.-F., Hopfensitz, A., & De Neys, W. (2013). The modular nature of
trustworthiness detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 143–150.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028930.
Bowles, S. (2009). Did warfare among ancestral hunter-gatherers affect the evolution of
human social behaviors? Science, 324, 1293–1298. https://doi.org/10.1126/
science.1168112.
Brotherton, R., & French, C. C. (2015). Intention seekers: Conspiracist ideation and
biased attributions of intentionality. PLoS One, 10, e0124125. https://doi.org/
10.1371/journal.pone.0124125.
Brotherton, R., French, C. C., & Pickering, A. D. (2013). Measuring belief in conspiracy
theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 279.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279.
Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring
individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures:
Conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 225. https://doi.org/
10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225.
Cairns, R. (2016). Climates of suspicion: ‘Chemtrail’ conspiracy narratives and the
international politics of geoengineering. Geographical Journal, 182, 70–84. https://
doi.org/10.1111/geoj.12116.
Campellone, T. R., & Kring, A. M. (2013). Who do you trust? The impact of facial emotion
and behaviour on decision making. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 603–620. https://doi.
org/10.1080/02699931.2012.726608.
Cox, J. C. (2004). How to identify trust and reciprocity. Games and Economic Behavior, 46
(2), 260–281. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0899-8256(03)00119-2.
Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., Denovan, A., & Parton, M. (2015). Conspiracy
theory and cognitive style: A worldview. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 206. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00206.
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2015). Climate change: Why the conspiracy theories are
dangerous. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists, 71, 98–106. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0096340215571908.
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J., & Harvey, A. J. (2016).
Someone is pulling the strings: Hypersensitive agency detection and belief in
conspiracy theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 22, 57–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/
13546783.2015.1051586.
Einstein, K. L., & Glick, D. M. (2015). Do I think BLS data are BS? The consequences of
conspiracy theories. Political Behavior, 37, 679–701. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s11109-014-9287-z.
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (1999). Test anxiety and the hierarchical model of
approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76, 628–644. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.628.
Engel, C. (2011). Dictator games: A meta study. Experimental Economics, 14(4), 583–610.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-011-9283-7.
Evans, A. M., & Revelle, W. (2008). Survey and behavioral measurements of
interpersonal trust. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1585–1593. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.07.011.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical
power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior
Research Methods, 39, 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146.
Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., von Rosenbladt, B., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2002).
A nation-wide laboratory: Examining trust and trustworthiness by integrating
behavioral experiments into representative surveys. Journal of Applied Social Science
Studies (Schmollers Jahrbuch), 122, 519–542.
Fleeson, W., & Leicht, C. (2006). On delineating and integrating the study of variability
and stability in personality psychology: Interpersonal trust as illustration. Journal of
Research in Personality, 40, 5–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.004.
Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J. L., Savin, N. E., & Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple
bargaining experiments. Games and Economic Behavior, 6(3), 347–369. https://doi.
org/10.1006/game.1994.1021.
Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed
disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355–379. https://
doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000527.
Glaeser, E. L., Laibson, D. I., Scheinkman, J. A., & Soutter, C. L. (2000). Measuring trust.
The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115, 811–846. https://doi.org/10.1162/
003355300554926.
”